Anatomy of a scream: What’s the science behind a shriek?

A new study explains how screams, through special acoustic properties, fulfil a unique biological niche.

In this undated photo provided by Sotheby’s Auction House in New York, “The Scream” by Edvard Munch is shown.

AP Photo/Sotheby’s Auction House

July 17, 2015

Of all the sounds humans are capable of producing, a scream tends to get the most attention. A universal signal for extreme distress. But what’s the science behind the scream?

According to a new study, screams make use of a sonic quality called “roughness,” which activates a neural response relating to fear. Neuroscientists David Poeppel and Luc Arnal also found that screams occupied an acoustic niche, not shared by other human vocalizations. They published their findings Thursday in the journal Current Biology.

On the surface, screams are a simple concept. They are loud, high-pitched, and intended to convey extreme distress or danger. But conventional wisdom aside, the scientific community never settled on a concrete definition, nor did it explain our responses to screaming.

In Kentucky, the oldest Black independent library is still making history

This came as a surprise to Dr. Arnal, a researcher at the University of Geneva, and Dr. Poeppel, a professor of cognitive neuroscience at New York University. They conducted a “slightly unconventional” acoustic study, collecting a host of different human and non-human sounds. Another group listened to the sounds, rating them based on how alarming they were.

As it turns out, a true scream isn’t characterized by volume or pitch. The real key is an acoustic feature called roughness. When the frequency of a sound modulates more quickly than our ears can differentiate, it is considered “rough” and we perceive it as unpleasant. Hearing very rough sounds is correlated with activity in the brain's amygdala, a region associated with feelings of fear. Screams, along with dissonant chords and artificial alarm sounds, all fell within the “roughness domain.”

“The more such roughness modulation a sound has, the more scary it seems – and the more effectively it activates the amygdala,” Poeppel explains.

Poeppel and colleagues also found that screams occupied a “privileged acoustic niche” – completely separate from the frequencies found in speech and gender-associated modulations.

“It means that that region of sound – in this case roughness – is not used by other communication signals,” Poeppel says. “That allows screams to have a high degree of specificity, and therefore efficacy, in eliciting a fear response.”

A majority of Americans no longer trust the Supreme Court. Can it rebuild?

But humans aren’t the only animals that scream. It can be remarkably difficult to distinguish caterwauling household feline, for example, from a screaming child. Arnal and Poeppel both agree that other animal sounds warrant similar studies.

“As a followup of this study we would like to apply our analyses to animal screams to learn how much this trait is conserved across species,” Arnal says. “Our primary guess is that screams are largely shared between mammals, but also perhaps birds and other animals. It would be of great interest to see whether roughness features are used to warn conspecifics in other species, and if the brain machinery that is recruited by these signals is also shared between animals.”

But Arnal and Poeppel’s findings are already significant. Screams aside, their research could have major implications for the study of human vocalization. It may even hint at the origins of vocal communication, Arnal says.

“There are some fun practical applications,” Poeppel says. “By understanding how screams work from an acoustic point of view, we can generate better alarm signals – and of course, more scary screams.”

“But the real insights are in the basic science,” he adds. “We are interested in identifying how communication is organized. These studies show us that screams are the kind of communicative signal that occupy a very reserved part of sounds, so they do not get mixed up with other kind of signals.”