Peacock-feather physics: How this "train-rattling" display might woo potential mates

When a peacock wants to attract a mate, he shakes his tail feathers – but that's not as simple as it sounds, scientists discovered.

|
Courtesy of Roslyn Dakin
A male peacock courts a female peacock (also called a peahen). To shake his enormous train, a peacock strums the train feathers with his tail feathers in a particular rhythm.
|
Courtesy of Roslyn Dakin
A peacock fans his train feathers out. When he shakes his train, microstructures in the eyespots make it look like the eyes are holding steady against a shimmering backdrop, scientists recently discovered.
|
Courtesy of Roslyn Dakin
The eyespots on a peacock's train feathers have microstructures that keep them locked in shape during the bird's train-rattling mating display.

A peacock's efforts to woo a mate can be downright feather-shaking. 

When the colorful birds try to attract a female, they shake and rattle their fanned-out train feathers, a motion that shows off the shimmering, iridescent eyespots that dot their spectacular train. But until recently, scientists knew little about the physics of this iconic display.

It turns out, peacocks use their shorter tail-feathers to strum the long train feathers like a guitar, creating vibrations at resonance. These vibrations, combined with tiny structures within the feathers, help create the illusion that the feathery eyespots are hovering in place over a moving background, according to a paper published Wednesday in the journal PLOS ONE.

"When the peahen is close to a peacock, his huge fan of feathers could practically fill her field of view," study author Roslyn Dakin, a researcher at the University of British Columbia, tells The Christian Science Monitor in an email. "And when he starts to wiggle those feathers, most of what she would see is moving, except for the eyespots floating on top."

How exactly does he do that?

A peacock makes his eye-spotted feathery fan quiver in a "train-rattling" display by wagging his tail. As his tail feathers strum across his train feathers, the whole fan vibrates. 

But this shaking doesn't spread uniformly across these long feathers. When the research team stuck the feathers under a microscope, they found that microhooks keep the peacocks' iconic eyespots latched together through this dance. The feather filaments around the eyespots sway along to the strumming while the center of the eyespots remain more stationary.

This makes the eyespots pop out as more iridescent, something Dr. Dakin's previous research found correlated with the amount of matings a peacock was able to achieve. 

The team also found that this train-rattling occurs at resonance, meaning the strumming that keeps the tail-feathers shaking happens at a frequency close to or at the feathers' natural frequency. 

It's like pushing a child on a swing, explains another study author, Suzanne Amador Kane, an applied physicist at Haverford College, in a phone interview with the Monitor.

"If you just pull them back and let them go, they swing at a pre-set frequency. The rate at which they swing back and forth is set by the mechanical properties of the swing and not how you're pushing them," she says. "So if you want to push them effectively and not waste your energy, you give them one push every time they swing back toward you."

When you're pushing them in sync with that natural frequency, that's resonance, Dr. Amador Kane explains. So the peacocks are strumming their tail feathers at just the right rhythm to match their train feathers' natural frequency, which is dictated by the physical properties of those feathers.

This resonance creates a reverberating mechanical sound, but it also suggests that this display is energetically efficient.

The researchers studied this train-rattling display by capturing high-speed video footage of peacocks during mating season. Then, they shook individual feathers in the lab at different frequencies to determine their natural frequency. That's how they determined the feathers were strummed at resonance.

This study illustrates that "the display is not a simple showing and gentle shaking of the feathers," writes Rebecca Kimball, an evolutionary biologist at the University of Florida who was not part of this study, in an email to the Monitor.

The rhythmic performance seems to be an attempt to attract a peahen's attention, but just what the females see in the biomechanical feat is still unknown.

"It could be that the males with the most powerful muscles perform the best displays. Or, it could be that males with the best endurance, or the most energy to spend, perform the best. Yet another possibility is that the peahens don't really care about vibration – maybe it gets their attention, but they don't use it when deciding who to mate with," Dakin says. "It could all just be noise, as Darwin suggested."

Dr. Kimball suggests that understanding the biomechanics could help "drive new studies that do a more critical assessment of what females look for in males."

"Peacocks have been the focus of much study due to their impressive feathers and the visual display of their tail which impresses all of us. However, previous research has focused on the size, number, and color of the feathers," she says, but this study suggests that "there may be much more complexity in what females look at in these displays than has been recognized previously."

"While this study cannot address why there may be so many different types of signals in peafowl, it emphasizes that even for species we think of as 'well-studied' we still have a lot to learn," Kimball says. 

You've read  of  free articles. Subscribe to continue.
Real news can be honest, hopeful, credible, constructive.
What is the Monitor difference? Tackling the tough headlines – with humanity. Listening to sources – with respect. Seeing the story that others are missing by reporting what so often gets overlooked: the values that connect us. That’s Monitor reporting – news that changes how you see the world.

Dear Reader,

About a year ago, I happened upon this statement about the Monitor in the Harvard Business Review – under the charming heading of “do things that don’t interest you”:

“Many things that end up” being meaningful, writes social scientist Joseph Grenny, “have come from conference workshops, articles, or online videos that began as a chore and ended with an insight. My work in Kenya, for example, was heavily influenced by a Christian Science Monitor article I had forced myself to read 10 years earlier. Sometimes, we call things ‘boring’ simply because they lie outside the box we are currently in.”

If you were to come up with a punchline to a joke about the Monitor, that would probably be it. We’re seen as being global, fair, insightful, and perhaps a bit too earnest. We’re the bran muffin of journalism.

But you know what? We change lives. And I’m going to argue that we change lives precisely because we force open that too-small box that most human beings think they live in.

The Monitor is a peculiar little publication that’s hard for the world to figure out. We’re run by a church, but we’re not only for church members and we’re not about converting people. We’re known as being fair even as the world becomes as polarized as at any time since the newspaper’s founding in 1908.

We have a mission beyond circulation, we want to bridge divides. We’re about kicking down the door of thought everywhere and saying, “You are bigger and more capable than you realize. And we can prove it.”

If you’re looking for bran muffin journalism, you can subscribe to the Monitor for $15. You’ll get the Monitor Weekly magazine, the Monitor Daily email, and unlimited access to CSMonitor.com.

QR Code to Peacock-feather physics: How this
Read this article in
https://www.csmonitor.com/Science/2016/0428/Peacock-feather-physics-How-this-train-rattling-display-might-woo-potential-mates
QR Code to Subscription page
Start your subscription today
https://www.csmonitor.com/subscribe