Did fear of vengeful gods encourage early human cooperation?

New research suggests a link between belief in moralistic, all-knowing, punitive gods and the propensity for prosocial behavior.

|
Courtesy of A. Wasil
Five killer whales pull Neptune in a 2003 artist's conception of a civic fountain proposed for San Diego's waterfront.

Unlike many animals, humans are part of complex social groups that extend beyond familial ties. But we didn't start out that way. 

Scientists, historians, and philosophers have long puzzled over how we came to have such large, complex societies. Some researchers have suggested that religion played a role in connecting people.

In fact, belief systems involving an all-knowing, moralistic, punitive god may provide particular motivation for cooperation among people who wouldn't necessarily have reason to trust each other, according to new research published Wednesday in the journal Nature.

The idea is that people will behave in a more prosocial manner if they feel like they're being watched, and that is compounded if there is a fear of punishment. So people might behave less selfishly if they believe a deity that rewards good behavior and punishes bad is always watching, even if that so-called good behavior seems to go against their self-interest.

This is not a new idea, says Peter Richerson, a cultural evolution researcher who was not associated with the study. "There's an impressive amount of evidence from several different lines that moralizing high gods have an effect on people's behavior."

So a team of researchers set out to test the idea.

Researchers presented 591 test subjects in different small societies around the world with an economic game. 

Players had two cups, a stack of coins and a six-sided die with three sides one color and three another. One cup was for themselves and the other was for another person with the same beliefs – someone far away, who they were unlikely to ever meet. 

To play, participants were instructed to think about which cup they wanted to put a coin in. Then they tossed the die. One color indicated that they put the coin in the cup they chose, while the other meant it went in the opposite cup.

Once the game was explained, the researchers left the players alone in a room. 

In theory, there was a 50-50 chance of the coin ending up in a given cup. But, as nobody was watching the participants, they could slip the coin into their own cup with nobody being the wiser. "So they can bend the rules effectively for their own gain," study lead author Benjamin Grant Purzycki tells the Monitor.

The scientists also ran a second round of the game with one cup representing someone from a geographically distant location who shared the same religion and the other being an anonymous local person who also shared the same religion.

At the end of the game, the money actually went to the player, one of their local community members, or a distant stranger of the same religion. So you'd expect someone to cheat and slide more money into the cup that was for themselves or a local community member because it would somehow benefit them. 

While some participants did cheat, it turns out that the players who believed in an all-knowing, punishing deity, came closest to reaching that 50-50 result, with an equal number of coins going in the stranger's cup as the familiar one.

So how could this silly little game explain the expansion of human societies?

Someone who is more likely to act generously to a distant stranger may also be more likely to engage with them on a different level, Dr. Purzycki says. "What our results suggest is that if they share the same kinds of beliefs about these types of deities, that might actually expedite or facilitate more effective and reliable trade networks or intergroup cooperation" or other such social networks.

Dr. Richerson cautions against assuming that it's only the fear of punishment driving prosocial behavior. There may be other factors contributing. "It may be that moralizing high gods are associated with stronger institutions rather than being scared of going to hell," he tells the Monitor. Perhaps "social systems that have those gods also have stronger institutions that cause people to behave better."

"Religion is certainly having an effect, but just exactly how it works," he says, "is still up in the air."

The question of how human groups went from small, kinship-based groups to the sprawling societies we have today has puzzled researchers. At the time, humans were competing over scarce resources, "so trying to understand and explain human cooperation has been a really big problem evolutionarily," says Kelly Clark, a researcher at the Kaufman Interfaith Institute at Grand Valley State University in Grand Rapids, Mich., who was not involved in the study.

"How can you trust someone who isn't in your small group?" he says. And it appears that a religious bond may serve a similar purpose as a blood bond.

But other researchers have suggested that religion came later, once humans were less worried about food and other resources. 

A study published last year suggests that moral religions arose when people had a reliable source of 20,000 calories of energy a day for food, livestock feed, fuel, and other key resources.

"This number appears to correspond with a certain peace of mind," lead author of that study, Nicolas Baumard, a research scientist at École Normale Supérieure in Paris, told Scientific American then. "Having a roof over your head, not feeling like the world is full of predators and enemies, knowing that you'll have enough to eat tomorrow."

You've read  of  free articles. Subscribe to continue.
Real news can be honest, hopeful, credible, constructive.
What is the Monitor difference? Tackling the tough headlines – with humanity. Listening to sources – with respect. Seeing the story that others are missing by reporting what so often gets overlooked: the values that connect us. That’s Monitor reporting – news that changes how you see the world.

Dear Reader,

About a year ago, I happened upon this statement about the Monitor in the Harvard Business Review – under the charming heading of “do things that don’t interest you”:

“Many things that end up” being meaningful, writes social scientist Joseph Grenny, “have come from conference workshops, articles, or online videos that began as a chore and ended with an insight. My work in Kenya, for example, was heavily influenced by a Christian Science Monitor article I had forced myself to read 10 years earlier. Sometimes, we call things ‘boring’ simply because they lie outside the box we are currently in.”

If you were to come up with a punchline to a joke about the Monitor, that would probably be it. We’re seen as being global, fair, insightful, and perhaps a bit too earnest. We’re the bran muffin of journalism.

But you know what? We change lives. And I’m going to argue that we change lives precisely because we force open that too-small box that most human beings think they live in.

The Monitor is a peculiar little publication that’s hard for the world to figure out. We’re run by a church, but we’re not only for church members and we’re not about converting people. We’re known as being fair even as the world becomes as polarized as at any time since the newspaper’s founding in 1908.

We have a mission beyond circulation, we want to bridge divides. We’re about kicking down the door of thought everywhere and saying, “You are bigger and more capable than you realize. And we can prove it.”

If you’re looking for bran muffin journalism, you can subscribe to the Monitor for $15. You’ll get the Monitor Weekly magazine, the Monitor Daily email, and unlimited access to CSMonitor.com.

QR Code to Did fear of vengeful gods encourage early human cooperation?
Read this article in
https://www.csmonitor.com/Science/2016/0211/Did-fear-of-vengeful-gods-encourage-early-human-cooperation
QR Code to Subscription page
Start your subscription today
https://www.csmonitor.com/subscribe